The Panopticon Effect


This post is part of the series: The Debriefing. Click to read all posts in this series.


So at my most recent conference there were a lot of research presentations. One of the fascinating things that comes up in clinical studies of diseases that are self-managed, and which was highlighted on several slides, is something I’ve come to call the panopticon effect. It might have a proper name, but if so, I haven’t heard it. The idea is fairly simple, and fairly obvious. For every study that has a control group, almost always, the control group shows better outcomes than the statistical averages.

In cases where control groups receive a placebo treatment, this discrepancy can be attributed to the placebo effect. But the effect persists even when there is no intervention whatsoever. It seems that merely being enrolled in a study is enough to create an increase in whatever outcome is being measured over what would normally be expected.
This could be a subtler extension of the placebo effect. We are constantly finding that placebo, mindfulness, and the like, while never substitutes for actual treatment, do have a measurable positive impact. But there is probably a simpler solution: these people know they are being watched. Even when data is anonomized, and there are no consequences for bad outcomes, there is still the pressure of being under surveillance.   And I suspect it has to do with an obligation that study participants feel to be worthy of the research being conducted.
I have heard variations on this theme slipped subtly in to enough different discussions that I have started to cue in on it lately. It is an idea similar to the ones raised over the obligations that patients often feel to fundraise and advocate on behalf of the organizations that bankroll research for their diseases; not mere camaraderie between people with shared experiences, but a sense of guilt for receiving tangential benefits from others’ work.
To briefly repeat what I have said in previous Debriefing articles: this mindset is embedded deep in the collective psyche of the communities with which I have experience, and in some instances is actively exploited by charity and advocacy organizations. The stereotype of sick and disabled being egregiously kindhearted and single-mindedly dedicated to fundraising and/or advocacy is both a cause and effect of this cycle. The same is naturally true of attention from healthcare professionals and researchers.
Frequent patients, especially in the United States, are constantly reminded of the scarcity of help. In every day-long phone call with insurance, in every long wait in the triage room, and every doctor visit cut short because appointments are scheduled back to back months in advance, we are reminded that what we need is in high demand and short supply. We are lucky to be able to get what we need, and there are plenty of others that are not so fortunate. Perhaps, on paper, we are entitled to life liberty, and the pursuit of happiness; to a standard of healthcare and quality of life; but in reality, we are privileged to get even as little as we do.
There is therefore great pressure to be deserving of the privileges we have received. To be worthy of great collective effort that has gone into keeping us alive. This is even more true where research is concerned; where the attention of the world’s brightest minds and taxpayer dollars are being put forth in a gamble to advance the frontiers of humanity. Being part of these efforts is something that is taken extremely seriously by many patients. For many of them, who are disqualified from military service and unable to perform many jobs unaided, contributing to scientific research is the highest calling they can answer.
This pressure manifests itself in many different ways. In many, it inspires an almost religious zeal; in others, it is a subtler, possibly even unconscious, response. In some cases, this pressure to live up to the help given by others stokes rebellion, displayed either as antisocial antipathy or even self harming tendencies. No one I have ever spoken to on the matter has yet failed to describe this pressure or agree that it exists in their life.
Moreover, the effect seems to be self reinforcing; the more attention a person receives, the more they feel an obligation to repay it, often through volunteering in research. This in turn increases the amount of attention received, and so on. As noted, participation in these studies seems to produce a statistically significant positive impact in whatever is being measured, completely divorced from any intervention or placebo effect.
We know that people behave differently when they feel they are being watched, and even more so when they feel that the people watching have expectations. We also know that prolonged stress, such as the stress of having to keep up external appearances over an extended period, take a toll, both psychologically and physiologically, on the patient. We must therefore ask at what cost this additional scrutiny, and the marginal positive impact on health results, comes.
We will probably never have a definitive answer to these sorts of questions. The  intersection of chronic physical conditions and mental health is convoluted, to say the least. Chronic health issues can certainly add additional stress and increase risk of mental illness, yet at the same time, make it harder to isolate and treat. After all, can you really say a person is unreasonably anxious when they worry about a disease that is currently killing them? In any case, if we are not likely to ever know for sure the precise effects of these added stresses, then we should at least commit to making them a known unknown.

Published by

Renaissance Guy (Mobile)

This account is the one I use to post from mobile. Same guy though.